Aesthetic
Perception in Design
(Developing a Product Language)
Email: heng-feng.zuo@solent.ac.uk
Email: mark.jones@solent.ac.uk
Mr Qiu
Song, MA, BA
Academy of Art
& Design, Tsinghua University, China
Email: greenth@sohu.com
Ugly things are
hard to sell. Aesthetic designs are often perceived as easier to use. This project has been set up to explore
the extent to which young designers are able to manipulate form, styling and
create an overall perception of a positive aesthetic. At the same time, we aim
to explore the DNA of a product¡¯s aesthetics and develop a linguistic system ¨C
product language. This system will differ from the traditional ¡®Semiotics¡¯ or
¡®Semantics¡¯, and although it will include these aspects, it will probe deeper
into the elements of formal aesthetics such as the shape, colour, material,
texture, proportion, dimensions, space, etc. This language system will be a
combination of both formal/external presentation and the
representative/embedded meanings of a physical product. It will enable more
effective communication between the various people involved in the product
development processes and in particular, the relationship between designer and
consumer. We have run a series of practice-based design workshops for
undergraduate design students both in the UK and China. This paper will
showcase the stage results from this workshop.
What makes
a product aesthetically appealing? This is an
old topic but always triggers new debates in the design field. Aesthetics, usually defined as the branch of philosophy that deals with
questions of beauty and artistic taste [1], has been recognised since antiquity
and has continually evolved over time. The word beauty is commonly applied to things
that are pleasing to the senses, imagination and/or understanding. It is often
what an artist or a designer endeavours to achieve in their works, either for
personal or mass interest and pleasure.
Aesthetics might have
different connotations if envisaged from different perspectives, such as
functional aesthetics, technological aesthetics, formal aesthetics,
psychological and cultural aesthetics etc [2], in addition to the sensory
aesthetics, which is the fundamental element of aesthetics. Though, an
aesthetic design may not have all these connotations at the same time. However,
it is widely agreed by scholars that sensory perception plays an intrinsic role
in aesthetic experience [3, 4, 5]. In other words, aesthetic experience starts
from pleasing the senses in the first instance. It has even been argued that
aesthetic experience is restricted to the (dis)pleasure that results from
sensory perception [5]. Actually, we can perhaps say that every experience
starts from our senses, as our sensory organs serve as the windows through
which human beings are able to know and feel the external world, but not all
experiences can be attributed to aesthetic experience. This implies that
sensation is not the only element of aesthetics. Although it might represent the
dominant one it does not represent the whole aesthetic experience. It is the
door through which we enter into the experience of beauty; Individual isolated
stimuli, either a colour, a sound, or a smell, can elicit physiological
response (e.g., comfort or excitement) such as represented by the change of
pulse, blood pressure. However, this cannot equal aesthetic response unless it
evokes our emotions.
You might say that you
find a particular curve, line or a colour to be beautiful, even when separated
from any context. However, there will be something underlying your instinctive
response to these stimuli that will share an association with an image or
meaning you will have stored in your memory, no matter how vague the
recollection. For example, the colour of green might remind you of freshness,
purity, hope, or the curvaceous lines resemble organic lives or the form of a
beautiful etc. This can be termed as ¡®association¡¯.
Association
plays a part in the process of aesthetic experience, and is connected with the
formal aspect of an artwork or designed product. Fundamental forms are given meaning through association with previous
knowledge of the world stored in long-term memory [6]. With
certain associations, meanings and emotions added to the primary sensory
experience, could the overall aesthetic experience be enriched to a greater
extent?
Exploring the aesthetic
association with designed products is one of the purposes of this research.
2. Product
language
No matter what kind of
aesthetic experience takes place in our mind, through the sensory interface
with any designed product, we often need to express this experience and
communicate our thoughts with others (designers, engineers, consumers, etc), in order to understand and generate good product aesthetics
and perception. This ¡®expression of
information¡¯ can be used as reference point when developing any new product.
For such expression and communication, we need a tool/dialogue through which we
can work ¨C product language.
In
our understanding, product language can speak about product¡¯s functions, forms,
style, aesthetics, value, culture, personality, etc. Product language was ever
said to have two main constituents: the formal and
the semiotic [7, 8]. The term ¡®semiotics¡¯ derives from the linguistics, deals
with the study of signs [7]. Another similar term also deriving from
linguistics is semantics, which deals with the study of meanings [8, 9].
Product semiotics and product semantics, literally, deals with the signs and
meanings of the product. However, this tends to focus more on the symbolic and
representative aspect. Product semiotics and
semantics might not always speak of aesthetics [9, P151], although there is a connection. For example, they share
some commonality when addressing the symbolic /representative meanings or
associations of the product. The adoption of the term product language
is based on the purpose of covering a wider range of information that a product
can deliver per se. Not just the symbolic and representative meanings, but also
firstly its formal aesthetic features via the sensory routes and thereafter the
connection between the formal aesthetic features and the
symbolic/representative meanings. However, there is little evidence to suggest
that in-depth research in the field of formal aesthetics has been conducted,
despite design researchers having taken a large interest in the product
language¡¯s semiotic aspects. There is the potential for combining product
semiotics and formal aesthetic features in order to establish a more complete
product language system [8].
In this research, our
second aim is to explore the ¡®product language¡¯. Initial research was conducted
to see if there is any common vocabulary used by people to describe a product¡¯s
aesthetics. Also of importance are the associations the product would carry,
and the possible correlation between the formal elements and the associations.
This could helpfully contribute to establish a sort of formal DNA for a product
or group of products. This may serve as the reference point for the design and
development of any new member to that family of products.
3. Preliminary
study of aesthetic description
The method for a pilot
study was asking people to give their verbal description of product aesthetics.
At this stage, we are not going to distinguish which descriptors can be
attributed to the aspect of formal aesthetics or the semiotic aspect of a
product. We will try to look at this division and a possible correlation
between these two aspects in a later stage. We used 10 top products that had
already been selected by an international panel of judges, representing those
products that were worthy of an international design award and having strongly
aesthetic appeal - Hannover, 2005 International Forum (IF) Design awards (see
Figure 1). These products represented different product areas such as medical,
domestic, technological, industrial etc and were selected as the products that
would be used for product description. 113 completed questionnaires were
collected from design students at Southampton Solent University. We presented
students with a list of pre-selected vocabulary for their reference (see Table
1). However, participants were also encouraged to use their own descriptive
words.
From the results we
found two phenomena. One, different products may share similar aesthetic
properties. Secondly, these described aesthetic properties cover both formal
aspect and symbolic aspect or associations, and the formal aesthetic
descriptions are correlated to some extent with the associations.
Usually, it seems
difficult to find aesthetic properties to fit all design artefacts, and there
is no sense in trying to apply the aesthetic features of one product to another
[9, P.151]. Nevertheless, this does not mean that different products should not
have some commonality in the expression of aesthetic properties. It is this
very commonality or similarity in aesthetic features, even if this commonality
can be quite limited, that can be applied as a reference when considering the
design and aesthetic of a new product. The widely used mood-board is a good
example of this.
Figure
1 Top 10 designs presented to design students for aesthetic evaluation
Figure
2 the aesthetic description of three different products
Figure 2, as an
example, shows that the aesthetic descriptors ¡®pure¡¯, ¡®architectural¡¯, and
¡®geometrical¡¯ are shared by three different products (a bathtub, a MP3, and a
bench). For a direct and simple understanding, we may regard the descriptor
¡®geometrical¡¯ as the description of shape, which is an element of formal
aesthetics; whist ¡®architectural¡¯ seems to be the description of an association
or metaphor, which has more sense of semiotic property. The descriptor ¡®pure¡¯
can be perceived as a visual simplicity (with the opposite as ¡®noisy¡¯ or
¡®complicated¡¯). It is hard to say that the description of ¡®pure¡¯ is completely
a formal aesthetic feature because when we say something is pure, that includes
your emotional feeling of appreciation. In other words, verbal description
cannot always make a clear division between the formal aesthetics and semiotic
meaning. A further statistical analysis revealed that these three descriptors
are correlated to a certain extent (with the correlation efficient r³0.5) under this research context.
Another example of such
a correlation has been shown between the descriptors of ¡®harmonious¡¯,
¡®delicate¡¯, ¡®organic¡¯, and ¡®curvaceous¡¯. Again, here ¡®curvaceous¡¯ may
completely address the formal aspect ¨C shape; whilst ¡®organic¡¯ integrates an
association between the product form and the life forms found in nature,
whether the human body, types of animals, or a drop of water, usually can be
¡®delicate¡¯ and ¡®curvaceous¡¯. Accordingly, it is easy to understand that these
natural forms are correlated with ¡®harmonious¡¯ as they reflect the results of
natural evolution.
It is worth conducting
further research to explore these aesthetic descriptors and their correlations
in a deeper level. And to see, when product context changes, how these descriptors
and correlations change as well. As we have seen, although some products used
in this research share some commonality of aesthetic properties, this cannot be
taken as a universal principle. It is argued that specific product language and
their correlation might be different from, say electronic products, furniture,
and transport tools etc.
4. Student
Design Workshop and Evaluation
The third aim of this
research is to explore to what extent young designers are able to manipulate
form and aesthetics. This has been conducted by running a practice-based design
workshop, where students completed a series of exercises plus a six-week design
project (MP3 & Speaker Unit). The MP3 project and some of the exercises are
attributed to a top-down process, where targeted
aesthetic perception comes first and is then translated into the 3D forms
designed by students. Other exercises are attributed to a bottom-up process, where the students are
shown images of products (4 product categories and 50 images of different styled
products for each category), and asked to interpret the aesthetic features into
and make a judgement as to the product perception. The Workshop has been
conducted at Southampton Solent University (UK) and Tsinghua University (China)
respectively. Further analysis of the results will help reveal the extent to
which cultural influence may impact on the design aesthetic and the level to
which product language can be used cross culturally.
In this paper, we
present the completed MP3 & Speaker design project and the evaluation of
their aesthetic and associative features. The design brief for MP3 was based on
three groups of descriptive words regarding a product aesthetic. We used the
correlated descriptors found in the pilot study to constitute the groups.
However, we further modified the combination of the descriptive words as
follows.
Group 1: Pure, Architectural,
Geometrical, and Technical
Group 2: Curvaceous, Organic, and
Fun
Group 3: Graceful, Cheerful, and
Powerful
Within group 1, we give
an extra descriptor of ¡®technical¡¯. Within group 2, ¡®curvaceous¡¯ and ¡®organic¡¯
remain, but added with an extra descriptor ¡®fun¡¯. Within group 3, the three
descriptors, from the pilot study, do not show any correlation between each
other. Students are then asked to produce designs for the MP3 & Speaker
Unit in line with any of the three groups of aesthetic properties. These
deliberate arrangements of design brief aim to give more challenges for young
designers to manipulate and balance the formal elements (mainly form, colour
and surface finish), to match a particular aesthetic target group.
Figure 3 shows some of
the activities during the design workshop and some of the finished presentation
models of the MP3 and Speaker Unit.
Figure
3 the design workshop conducted in UK and China
Students were given
free choice as to which aesthetic group they were to produce designs for,
although we found that most students did elect for Group 1 or Group 2. The
evaluated results shown below compare the original aesthetic target, as
intended by the design students, with those that were perceived by an
independent group of students who conducted the evaluation of the finished
designs (shown in Figure 4).
(a)
(b)
Figure 4
the comparison between the evaluation and the original targets
It is clear that most
of the designs of MP3 & Speaker are perceived to have a combination of the
three groups of aesthetic features to some extent. However, the designs for
Group 1 have most effectively matched the aesthetic target: pure,
architectural, geometrical and technical (more than 70% matching, see the
marking points in Triangle (b) for Group 1 bunched around the bottom-right
corner). The designs for Group 2 (except for one design in this group) also
have matched the target fairly well: curvaceous, organic, and fun (more than
60% matching, see the marking points in Triangle (b) for Group 2 positioned
slightly away from the top corner). As to the designs for Group 3, only one
design was selected from the very few designs in this group. Furthermore, this
design was perceived to be within Group 1 rather than Group 3. At the same
time, one design from Group 2 has been perceived to have the aesthetic features
of Group 3.
The
above results seem to imply that certain ambiguity can occur when we try to
perceive the aesthetic features of a product, where the word associations have
less correlation, e.g., in this case, graceful, cheerful, and powerful. On the
other hand, the aesthetic features that have higher correlation appear easier
to match. We may borrow a hypothesis of processing fluency of aesthetics to
explain this. Rolf Reber and Norbert
Schwarz proposed
that aesthetic pleasure is a function of the perceiver¡¯s processing dynamics.
The more fluently perceivers can process an object, the more positive their
aesthetic responses (Rolf
Reber and Norbert Schwarz, 2004). In this research case, during either the
top-down process of design following aesthetic targets or the bottom-up
process of evaluation and perception of completed designs, the more fluently
perceivers can process aesthetic features, the more effectively these features
can be applied in designs and can be perceived. Group 1 and Group 2 have the
aesthetic descriptors correlated, whilst Group 3 have non-correlated
descriptors, which may address the reason why fewer students selected Group 3
as the target for design in the first instance, as there was possibility
greater ambiguity in this category.
5. Further
research
Further research work
includes three main aspects. Firstly, to expand the product language, including
the aesthetic descriptors, to a wider range of product categories, to see what
correlation between the descriptors can be drawn and to what extent the
commonality can be found. Secondly, on a micro scale, to find how the elements
of design form, such as shape, size, colour, materials and textures,
proportion, etc correspond to each of the descriptors in product language.
These two aspects are the fundamental milestones towards the establishment of a
product language system. Thirdly, further exploration focuses on whether the
above aspects can be influenced by cultural background. This part of research
is currently on going in parallel to the first two aspects. This programme of
collaborative research is being conducted between universities in UK, China,
and Italy.
Aesthetic
experience of a designed product starts from the sensory perception between the
product and users. Product language covers the description of formal aesthetics
and the description of associations the product carries and the symbolic or
representative meanings embedded in the product. These two aspects of
description in product language system can be correlated to a certain
extent. However, the boundaries
between these two aspects can sometimes become blurred when using verbal
description. Preliminary exploration suggests some correlation between the
descriptors such as ¡®pure-architectural-geometrical¡¯ and
¡®harmonious-delicate-organic-curvaceous¡¯. Young designers tend to differ in
their abilities when manipulating the form of product to match different
aesthetic targets. However, when the aesthetic features in one product are consistently
correlated, these greater abilities seem to be evident and are facilitated more
easily.
Acknowledgement
This project was supported by the
Centre for Advanced Scholarship in Art & Design - Capability Fund,
Southampton Solent University.
Thanks
are given to Professor Cai Jun, Professor Yan Yang, Academy of Art and Design,
Tsinghua University for the arrangements of collaborative design workshop.
1.
Catherine Soanes, Angus Stevenson, (2004), Concise
Oxford English Dictionary, 11th Edition, Oxford University
Press.
2.
Hengfeng Zuo, Mark Jones, (2005), Exploration into
formal aesthetics in design: (material) texture, in Proceeding of 8th
Generative Art Conference, Milan. P.160-171.
3.
Dewitt H. Parker, (2004), The Principles of
Aesthetics, 10th Edition
4.
Sven Inge Braten, (2001), A Framework for Analysis
of Dynamic Aesthetics,
http://www.ivt.ntnu.no/ipd/fag/PD9/2001/Artikler/Braaten_I.pdf
5.
Paul Hekkert, (2006), Design Aesthetics: Principles
of Pleasure in Design, Psychology
Science, Volume 48, (2),
P.157-172.
6.
Joseph G. Kickasola, Cinemediacy:
Theorizing an Aesthetic Phenomenon. Baylor University.
http://www.avila.edu/journal/kick.pdf
7.
Jochem Gros, Erweiterter Funktionalismus und
Empirische Aesthetik, Braunschweig, Germany 1973.
8.
Kristin H. Lower Gautvik, (2001), Towards A
Product Language: Theories and methodology regarding aesthetic analysis of
design products,
http://design.ntnu.no/forskning/artikler/2001/Gautvik_I.pdf
9.
Susann Vihma, (1995), Products As Representations:
a semiotic and semantic study of design products, University of Art and
Design Helsinki UIAH, UIAH Information and Publishing Unit.
10. Rolf Reber, Piotr Winkielman, (2004), Processing
Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver¡¯s Processing
Experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, vol.8, no.4,
P.364-382.